Histopathology image classification: Highlighting the gap between manual analysis and AI automation
Contents
A child prodigy is, technically, a child under the age of 10 who produces meaningful work in some domain at the level of an adult expert.[1][2][3] The term is also applied more broadly to describe young people who are extraordinarily talented in some field.[4]
The term wunderkind (from German Wunderkind; literally "wonder child") is sometimes used as a synonym for child prodigy, particularly in media accounts. Wunderkind also is used to recognise those who achieve success and acclaim early in their adult careers.[5]
Generally, prodigies in all domains are suggested to have relatively elevated IQ, extraordinary memory, and exceptional attention to detail. Significantly, while math and physics prodigies may have higher IQs, this may be an impediment to art prodigies.[6]
Examples
Chess prodigies
Deliberate practice
K. Anders Ericsson emphasised the contribution of deliberate practice over their innate talent to prodigies' exceptional performance in chess.[7] The deliberate practice is energy-consuming and requires attention to correct mistakes. As prodigies start formal chess training early with intense dedication to deliberate practice, they may accumulate enough deliberate practice for their exceptional performance. Therefore, this framework provide an arguably reasonable justification for chess prodigies. However, similar amounts of practice also make children differ in their achievements because of other factors such as the quality of deliberate practice, and their interests in chess.
Intelligence and chess performance
Chess prodigies may have higher IQs than normal children. This positive link between chess skills of prodigies and intelligence is particularly significant on the “performance intelligence”, regarding fluid reasoning, spatial processing, attentiveness to details, and visual-motor integration, while least significant on the “verbal intelligence”, regarding the ability to understand and reason using concepts framed in words.[8] However, this positive link is absent among adult experts. Remarkably, in the sample of chess prodigies, the more intelligent children played chess worse. This is considered as the result of less practice time of more intelligent chess skills.
Practice-plasticity-processes model
Practice-plasticity-processes (PPP) model was proposed to explain the existence of chess prodigies by integrating the practice extreme and innate talent extreme theories. Besides deliberate practice, neuroplasticity is identified as another critical component for developing chess heuristics (e.g., simple search techniques and abstract rules like “occupy the centre”), chunks (e.g., group of pieces locating in specific squares), and templates (e.g., familiarised complex patterns of chunks), which are essential for chess skills. The more plastic the brain is, the easier it is for them to acquire chunks, templates, and heuristics for better performance. On the other hand, inherited individual differences in the brain are circumscribed children to learn these skills. [9]
Music prodigies
Music prodigies usually express their talents in exceptional performance or composition.
The Multifactorial Gene-Environment Interaction Model incorporates the roles of adequate practice, certain personality traits, elevated IQ, and exceptional working memory in the explanation of music prodigies.[10] A study comparing current and former prodigies with normal people and musicians who showed their talents or were trained later in life to test this model. It found prodigies neither have exceptional performance in terms of IQ, working memory, nor specific personality. This study also emphasises the significance of frequent practice early in life, when the brain is more plastic. Besides the quality of practice, and the parental investment, the experience of flow during the practice is important for efficient and adequate practice for music prodigies. Practice demands high levels of concentration, which is hard for children in general, but flow can provide inherent pleasures of the practice to ensure this focused work.[11]
Memory capacity of prodigies
PET scans performed on several mathematics prodigies have suggested that they think in terms of[clarification needed] long-term working memory (LTWM).[12] This memory, specific to a field of expertise,[clarification needed] is capable of holding relevant information for extended periods, usually hours. For example, experienced waiters have been found to hold the orders of up to twenty customers in their heads while they serve them, but perform only as well as an average person in number-sequence recognition. The PET scans also answer questions about which specific areas of the brain associate themselves with manipulating numbers.[12]
One subject[who?] never excelled as a child in mathematics, but he taught himself algorithms and tricks for calculatory speed, becoming capable of extremely complex mental math. His brain, compared to six other controls, was studied using the PET scan, revealing separate areas of his brain that he manipulated to solve complex problems. Some of the areas that he and presumably prodigies use are brain sectors dealing in visual and spatial memory, as well as visual mental imagery. Other areas of the brain showed use by the subject, including a sector of the brain generally related to childlike "finger counting", probably used in his mind to relate numbers to the visual cortex.[12]
This finding is consistent with the introspective report of this[which?] calculating prodigy, which states that he used visual images to encode and retrieve numerical information in LTWM. Compared to short-term memory strategies, used by normal people on complex mathematical problems, encoding and retrieval episodic memory strategies would be more efficient. The prodigy may switch between these two strategies, which reduce the storage retrieval times of long-term memory and circumvent the limited capacities of short-term memory. In turn, they can encode and retrieve specific information (e.g., the intermediate answers during the calculation) in the long-term working memory more accurately and effectively.[13]
Similar strategies were found among prodigies mastering mental abacus calculation. The positions of beads on the physical abacus act as visual proxies of each digit for prodigies to solve complex computations. This one-to-one corresponding structure allows them to rapidly encode and retrieve digits in the long-term working memory during the calculation.[14] The fMRI scans showed stronger activation of brain areas related to visual processing for Chinese children being trained with abacus mental compared to control groups. This may indicate a greater demand for visuospatial information processing and visual-motor imagination in abacus mental calculation. Additionally, the right middle frontal gyrus activation is suggested to be the neuroanatomical link between prodigies’ abacus mental calculation and the visuospatial working memory. This activation serves a mediation effect on the correlation between abacus-based mental calculation and visuospatial working memory. A training-induced neuroplasticity regarding working memory performance for children is proposed.[15] A study examining German calculating prodigies also proposed a similar reason for exceptional calculation abilities. Excellent working memory capacities and neuroplastic changes brought by extensive practice would be essential to enhance this domain-specific skill.[16]
Working memory/cerebellum theory
"My mother said that I should finish high school and go to college first."
Noting that the cerebellum acts to streamline the speed and efficiency of all thought processes, Vandervert[18] explained the abilities of prodigies in terms of the collaboration of working memory and the cognitive functions of the cerebellum. Citing extensive imaging evidence, Vandervert first proposed this approach in two publications which appeared in 2003. In addition to imaging evidence, Vandervert's approach is supported by the substantial award-winning studies of the cerebellum by Masao Ito.[19]
Vandervert[20] provided extensive argument that, in the prodigy, the transition from visual-spatial working memory to other forms of thought (language, art, mathematics) is accelerated by the unique emotional disposition of the prodigy and the cognitive functions of the cerebellum. According to Vandervert, in the emotion-driven prodigy (commonly observed as a "rage to master") the cerebellum accelerates the streamlining of the efficiencies of working memory in its manipulation and decomposition/re-composition of visual-spatial content into language acquisition and into linguistic, mathematical, and artistic precocity.[21]
Essentially, Vandervert has argued that when a child is confronted with a challenging new situation, visual-spatial working memory and speech-related and other notational system-related working memory are decomposed and re-composed (fractionated) by the cerebellum and then blended in the cerebral cortex in an attempt to deal with the new situation.[22] In child prodigies, Vandervert believes this blending process is accelerated due to their unique emotional sensitivities which result in high levels of repetitious focus on, in most cases, particular rule-governed knowledge domains. He has also argued that child prodigies first began to appear about 10,000 years ago when rule-governed knowledge had accumulated to a significant point, perhaps at the agricultural-religious settlements of Göbekli Tepe or Cyprus.[23]
Development
Nature versus nurture
Some researchers believe that prodigious talent tends to arise as a result of the innate talent of the child, and the energetic and emotional investment that the child ventures. Others believe that the environment plays the dominant role, many times in obvious ways. For example, László Polgár set out to raise his children to be chess players, and all three of his daughters went on to become world-class players (two of whom are grandmasters), emphasising the potency a child's environment can have in determining the pursuits toward which a child's energy will be directed, and showing that an incredible amount of skill can be developed through suitable training.[24]
Co-incidence theory
Co-incidence theory explains the development of prodigies with a continuum of the discussion of nature and nurture. This theory states that the integrative of various factors in the development and expression of human potential, including:[25]
- Biological qualities (e.g., physical distinctions and limitations which entail talents, such as the absolute pitch of music prodigies)
- Individual psychological qualities (e.g., perseverance, patience, attention to detail)
- Intermediate context (e.g., family structure and its traditions)
- Cultural effect (e.g., an increasing number of chess prodigies may be due to the professionalization of chess, or the emergence of computers as a study tool)
- Historical and political context (e.g., girls in certain religious backgrounds may not receive training in music or any other art)
- Domain and the surrounding field (e.g., whether children with exceptional skills were born in an environment with flourishment in this field)
Late development
Prodigiousness in childhood is not always maintained into adulthood. Some researchers have found that gifted children fall behind due to lack of effort. Jim Taylor, professor at the University of San Francisco, theorizes that this is because gifted children experience success at an early age with little to no effort and may not develop a sense of ownership of success. Therefore, these children might not develop a connection between effort and outcome. Some children might also believe that they can succeed without effort in the future as well. Dr. Anders Ericcson, professor at Florida State University, researches expert performance in sports, music, mathematics, and other activities. His findings demonstrate that prodigiousness in childhood is not a strong indicator of later success. Rather, the number of hours devoted to the activity was a better indicator. [26]
Rosemary Callard-Szulgit and other educators have written extensively about the problem of perfectionism in bright children, calling it their "number one social-emotional trait". Gifted children often associate even slight imperfection with failure, so that they become fearful of effort, even in their personal lives, and in extreme cases end up virtually immobilized.[27]
Link with autism
Prodigies have been found with the over-representation of relatives with autism on their family pedigrees. Autism traits on the Autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) were reported in both first-degree relatives of child prodigies and of autism, which was higher than normal prevalence.[28]
Some autistic traits can be found among prodigies. Firstly, the social function of arithmetic prodigies may be weaker because of larger activation in certain brain areas enhancing their arithmetic performance, which is also essential for social and emotional functions (i.e., precuneus, lingual and fusiform gyrus). These neuroplastic changes in neural networks may modulate their social performances in terms of emotional face processing and emotional evaluation of complex social interactions. Nevertheless, this emotional or social modulation must not score at psychopathological levels.[16] Additionally, the attentiveness to details, a typical characteristic of AQ, is enhanced among prodigies compared to normal people, even those with Asperger syndrome.[6]
See also
- Chess prodigy
- Genius
- Gifted education
- Intellectual giftedness
- Late bloomer
- List of child music prodigies
- List of child prodigies
- List of fictional child prodigies
- Malleability of intelligence
- Polymath
- Savant syndrome
References
- ^ Feldman, David H.; Morelock, M. J. (2011). "Prodigies". In Runco, Mark A.; Pritzker, Steven R. (eds.). Prodigies. Encyclopedia of Creativity (Second ed.). Academic Press. pp. 261–265. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-375038-9.00182-5. ISBN 978-0-12-375038-9.
For the purposes of this and future research, a prodigy was defined as a child younger than 10 years of age who has reached the level of a highly trained professional in a demanding area of endeavor.
- ^ Rose, Lacey (2 March 2007). "Whiz Kids". Forbes. Retrieved 3 April 2015.
At the moment, the most widely accepted definition is a child, typically under the age of 10, who has mastered a challenging skill at the level of an adult professional.
- ^ Feldman, David Henry (Fall 1993). "Child prodigies: A distinctive form of giftedness". Gifted Child Quarterly. 27 (4): 188–193. doi:10.1177/001698629303700408. S2CID 144180264.
- ^ "Prodigy". Merriam-Webster. Retrieved 22 October 2021.
- ^ "wunderkind". Merriam-Webster. Retrieved 6 December 2012.
- ^ a b Ruthsatz, Joanne; Ruthsatz-Stephens, Kimberly; Ruthsatz, Kyle (May 2014). "The cognitive bases of exceptional abilities in child prodigies by domain: Similarities and differences". Intelligence. 44: 11–14. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.010. ISSN 0160-2896.
- ^ Ericsson, K. Anders; Krampe, Ralf T.; Tesch-Römer, Clemens (June 1993). "The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance". Psychological Review. 100 (3): 363–406. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.363. ISSN 1939-1471.
- ^ Frydman, Marcel; Lynn, Richard (May 1992). "The general intelligence and spatial abilities of gifted young Belgian chess players". British Journal of Psychology. 83 (2): 233–235. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1992.tb02437.x. ISSN 0007-1269. PMID 1611410.
- ^ Campitelli, Guillermo; Gobet, Fernand; Bilalić, Merim (3 June 2014), Simonton, Dean Keith (ed.), "Cognitive Processes and Development of Chess Genius: An Integrative Approach", The Wiley Handbook of Genius (1 ed.), Wiley, pp. 350–374, doi:10.1002/9781118367377.ch17, ISBN 978-1-118-36740-7, retrieved 19 March 2024
- ^ Ullén, Fredrik; Hambrick, David Zachary; Mosing, Miriam Anna (2016). "Rethinking expertise: A multifactorial gene–environment interaction model of expert performance". Psychological Bulletin. 142 (4): 427–446. doi:10.1037/bul0000033. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 26689084.
- ^ Marion-St-Onge, Chanel; Weiss, Michael W.; Sharda, Megha; Peretz, Isabelle (11 December 2020). "What Makes Musical Prodigies?". Frontiers in Psychology. 11. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566373. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 7759486. PMID 33362630.
- ^ a b c Butterworth, Brian (January 2001). "What makes a prodigy?". Nature Neuroscience. 4 (1): 11–12. doi:10.1038/82841. PMID 11135636. S2CID 14967746.
- ^ Pesenti, Mauro; Zago, Laure; Crivello, Fabrice; Mellet, Emmanuel; Samson, Dana; Duroux, Bruno; Seron, Xavier; Mazoyer, Bernard; Tzourio-Mazoyer, Nathalie (January 2001). "Mental calculation in a prodigy is sustained by right prefrontal and medial temporal areas". Nature Neuroscience. 4 (1): 103–107. doi:10.1038/82831. ISSN 1546-1726. PMID 11135652.
- ^ Ericsson, K. Anders; Kintsch, Walter (1995). "Long-term working memory". Psychological Review. 102 (2): 211–245. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.211. ISSN 1939-1471. PMID 7740089.
- ^ Wang, Chunjie; Xu, Tianyong; Geng, Fengji; Hu, Yuzheng; Wang, Yunqi; Liu, Huafeng; Chen, Feiyan (14 August 2019). "Training on Abacus-Based Mental Calculation Enhances Visuospatial Working Memory in Children". The Journal of Neuroscience. 39 (33): 6439–6448. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3195-18.2019. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6697396. PMID 31209171.
- ^ a b Fehr, Thorsten; Weber, Jochen; Willmes, Klaus; Herrmann, Manfred (April 2010). "Neural correlates in exceptional mental arithmetic—About the neural architecture of prodigious skills". Neuropsychologia. 48 (5): 1407–1416. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.007. PMID 20079753.
- ^ Charles McGrath, "Philosopher, 65, Lectures Not About 'What Am I?' but 'What Is I?'", 28 January 2006
- ^ Vandervert 2007, 2009a, 2009b[full citation needed]
- ^ Ito 2005, 2007[full citation needed]
- ^ Vandervert 2009a[full citation needed]
- ^ Vandervert 2009a, 2009b, in press-a, in press-b[full citation needed]
- ^ Vandervert, in press-a, in press-b.[full citation needed]
- ^ Vandervert, 2009a, 2009b, in press-c[full citation needed]
- ^ Queen takes all - Telegraph.co.uk, January 2002
- ^ Feldman, David Henry; Goldsmith, Lynn T. (1991). Nature's gambit: child prodigies and the development of human potential. Education and psychology of the gifted series. New York, NY: Teachers College Press. ISBN 978-0-8077-3143-7.
- ^ Taylor, Jim. "The Problem of Giftedness." Psychology Today, Sussex Publishers, 19 November 2009, www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-power-prime/200911/the-problem-giftedness.
- ^ Rosemary Callard-Szulgit, Perfectionism and Gifted Children. 2nd edition, R&L Education. 31 July 2012.
- ^ Ruthsatz, Joanne; Urbach, Jourdan B. (September 2012). "Child prodigy: A novel cognitive profile places elevated general intelligence, exceptional working memory and attention to detail at the root of prodigiousness". Intelligence. 40 (5): 419–426. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2012.06.002. ISSN 0160-2896.
Further reading
- Ellenberg, Jordan (30 May 2014). "The Wrong Way to Treat Child Geniuses". Wall Street Journal. Retrieved 1 June 2014.
- Hulbert, Ann (2018). Off the Charts: The Hidden Lives and Lessons of American Child Prodigies. Knopf. ISBN 978-1101947296.
- "How working memory and the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and innovation" (PDF). Creativity Research Journal.
- Robinson, Andrew (2010). Sudden Genius?: The Gradual Path to Creative Breakthroughs. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-956995-3.
External links
- Notebook: Child Prodigies on YouTube, "CBS News Online", 26 February 2010.